Henry Herbert Goddard
(14 August 1866-1957)
American Psychologist
Influences
Education
- B.A., Haverford College, Pennsylvania (1887)
- M.A. in Mathematics, Haverford College (1889)
- Ph.D. in Psychology, Clark University, Worcester, Massachusetts (1899)
Career
- Football coach and Instructor of Latin, history, and botany, University
of Southern California (1887-1888)
- Teacher, Damascus Academy, Ohio (1889-1891)
- Teacher and Principal, Oak Grove Seminary, Vassalboro, Maine, (1891-1896)
- Professor of Psychology and Pedagogy, State Normal School, West Chester,
Pennsylvania, (1899-1906)
- Director of Research, Training
School for Feeble-minded Girls and Boys, Vineland, NJ (1906-1918)
- Invited to Ellis Island assist in identifying mental defectives (1910,
1912)
- Member of Army Alpha and Beta Testing Team (1917-1919)
- Ohio State Bureau of Juvenile Research (1918-1938)
- Professor of Abnormal and Clinical Psychology, Ohio State University
(1922-1938)
Major Contributions
- Translated the Binet-Simon intelligence scale into English (1908)
- Distributed 22,000 copies of the translated Binet scale and 88,000
answer blanks across the United States (1908-1915)
- Established the first laboratory for the psychological study of mentally
retarded persons (1910)
- Helped to draft the first American law mandating special education
(1911)
- Strongly argued the hereditarian position
Definition of Intelligence
"
our thesis is that the chief determiner of human conduct
is a unitary mental process which we call intelligence: that this process
is conditioned by a nervous mechanism which is inborn: that the degree
of efficiency to be attained by that nervous mechanism and the consequent
grade of intelligence or mental level for each individual is determined
by the kind of chromosomes that come together with the union of the germ
cells: That it is but little affected by any later influences except such
serious accidents as may destroy part of the mechanism" (Goddard,
1920, p. 1).
Ideas and Interests
It is no exaggeration to characterize Henry Goddard as the father of
intelligence testing in the United States. His biographer points out that
he was either a leader or a participant in every significant event occurring
during the genesis of American psychometrics. In the years between 1908
and 1918 he translated the Binet-Simon
Intelligence Scale into English, distributed 22,000 copies of the test
throughout the United States, advocated for its use in the public schools,
established an intelligence testing program on Ellis Island, and served
as a member of Robert Yerkes' Army Alpha and
Beta testing team during World War One (Zenderland, 1998, p.2). Goddard's
contributions to public education were considerable as well: He helped
draft the first state law mandating that schools provide special education,
and stressed the need for public school reform by suggesting that normal
children could benefit from the instructional techniques originally developed
for use with retarded students (Zenderland, p. 124, 63).
When Goddard began working in education he was an unlikely candidate
for such a distinguished career. He spent his 20s working as a Quaker
schoolteacher and principal, and he didn't begin his Ph.D. work until
he was 30 years old. He graduated in 1899 and took a job teaching psychology
and pedagogy at a state normal school in Pennsylvania. In 1906 he was
offered a position in a small New Jersey institution called the Training
School for Feeble-Minded Girls and Boys. He enjoyed his work with the
students there, and became very interested in the both the causes of mental
deficiency and the teaching methods employed by the instructors. His research
facility at the school was perhaps the first laboratory for the scientific
study of mentally retarded persons.
In 1908 Goddard traveled to Europe and secured copies of the Binet-Simon
intelligence scales. Upon his return to the U.S., he translated the test
and began using it with the mentally retarded children living at the school.
Convinced of its effectiveness, he began distributing it widely across
the United States (Fancher, 1985; Zenderland, 1998). The fact that it
was Goddard who popularized the Binet scales offers an historical irony;
Binet was extremely careful not to attribute students' test performance
to any inherent or unchangeable factors. Goddard was a vociferous hereditarian.
(Fancher, 1985; Zenderland, 1998).
Goddard's views on intelligence were derived from Mendelian genetics.
He believed that feeblemindedness was caused by the transmission of a
single recessive gene. His 1912 book The
Kallikak Family: A Study in the Heredity of Feeble-mindedness purported
to prove this through an examination the differences between two branches
of a single family tree. (For More information about The Kallikak Family,
please see our related Hot Topic.)
Goddard was a eugenicist, and his views on population growth and control
were very similar to those of the Englishman Francis
Galton (1822-1911). Although both men were concerned with raising
their respective country's national intelligence, they differed in their
approach. Galton was more vocal about promoting population growth among
highly intelligent people, whereas Goddard was more focused on preventing
the breeding of feebleminded people (Fancher, 1984). Goddard believed
that compulsory sterilization would solve the American problem (Goddard,
1912, p. 106-107). However, he understood that many Americans would find
it offensive. As an alternative, he suggested that mentally deficient
individuals should be kept, humanely, in institutions:
Before considering any other method, the writer would insist
that segregation and colonization is not by any means as hopeless
a plan as it may seem to those who look only at the immediate increase
in the tax rate. If such colonies were provided in sufficient number
to take care of all the distinctly feeble-minded cases in the community,
they would very largely take the place of our present almshouses
and prisons, and they would greatly decrease the number in our insane
hospitals. Such colonies would save an annual loss in property and
life, due to the action of these irresponsible people, sufficient
to nearly, or quite, offset the expense of the new plant. Besides,
if these feeble-minded children were early selected and carefully
trained, they would become more or less self-supporting in their
institutions, so that the expense of their maintenance would be
greatly reduced (Goddard, 1912, p. 105-6).
Goddard's ideas were representative of the eugenicist zeitgeist in America
The American public had come to suspect that a disproportionately large
percentage of the new Ellis Island immigrants were mentally defective.
In 1882 the United States Congress had passed a law prohibiting mentally
defective people from passing through the Ellis Island checkpoint. Enforcing
this law proved to be difficult because as many as 5,000 immigrants needed
to be inspected each day. In 1910 Goddard was among those invited to Ellis
Island to investigate how the screening process might be expedited. In
1912 he returned to the Island, accompanied by two specially trained assistants.
The procedure he developed was a two-step process: One assistant would
visually screen for suspected mental defectives as the immigrants passed
through the checkpoint. These individuals would then proceed to another
location where the other assistant would test them with a variety of performance
measures and a revised version of the Binet scales. Goddard believed that
trained inspectors could be more accurate than the Ellis Island physicians;
the key to their success was expertise developed through experience, and
he likened the process to wine or tea-tasting (Zenderland, 1998, p. 268).
The number of immigrants who were deported increased exponentially as
a result of these screening measures (Zenderland, p. 273). (For more information
about Goddard's activities on Ellis Island, please see our related Hot
Topic.)
Selected Publications
Goddard, H. H. (1912). The
Kallikak Family: A study in the heredity of feeble-mindedness.
New York: Macmillan.
Goddard, H. H. (1914). Feeble-mindedness: Its causes and consequences:
New York: Macmillan.
Goddard, H. H. (1917). Mental tests and the immigrant. Journal of Delinquency,
2, 243-277.
Goddard, H.H. (1920). Human efficiency and levels of intelligence.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
References
Fancher, R.E. (1985). The intelligence men: Makers of the IQ controversy.
New York: W.W. Norton & Company
Goddard, H. H. (1912). The Kallikak family: A study in the heredity
of feeble-mindedness. New York: Macmillan.
Goddard, H.H. (1920). Human efficiency and levels of intelligence.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Strickland, B. (Ed). (2000). Kallikak family. In Gale encyclopedia
of education (2nd ed). (pp. 352-353). New York: Gale Group.
Zenderland, L. (1998). Measuring minds: Henry Herbert Goddard and
the origins of American intelligence testing. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Zuzne, L. (1984). Goddard, Henry Herbert. In Biographical dictionary
of psychology (pp. 158). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Image Courtesy of The Archives of the History of Psychology, The University
of Akron
Home | Interactive
Map | Alphabetic Index | Time
Period Index
Hot Topics | Map - PDF | References | Contributors | Comments
For further information please contact
Content questions: Dr. Jonathan
Plucker (jonathan.plucker AT uconn.edu)
Technical questions: Technology Co-Director (intelltheory AT gmail.com)
Copyright © 2013
Last Modified:
29 April 2018